This FAQ contains answers to questions concerning the following topics:
- Effect size calculations
- Meta-analytical techniques
- Confidence and prediction intervals
- Microsoft Excel and calculations
- Other topics
If your question is not included here, please contact us.
Effect size calculations
We need to account for the correlation between pre- and post treatment ‘performance’, because we end up with a wrong estimate of the precision of the combined effect size (Morris & DeShon, 2002). The problem is that the groups in the experimental design are not independent of each other and we need some method to account for this dependence. In the independent groups design, i.e., with as separate treatment and control group, the correlation between the outcomes is by definition zero. However, when the correlation is not equal to zero, the variance of the difference between the group outcomes is over (or under) estimated.
Oftentimes, the correlation between outcomes is not known for the study of interest. Then, researchers need to make an informed decision (based on other studies) or make a sensitivity analysis by comparing the results for different levels of the correlation. See also section 4.2 of the User Manual and (Borenstein et al., 2009. p. 232).
The total sample size (N) is the number of participants that participated in both the pre- and the posttest. And thus not the sum of participants in the pre- and posttest.
No problem! There are easy formulas you can use to derive the standard error of the effect size. See for example the online calculators of the Campbell Collaboration (based on Lipsey and Wilson, 2001): http://www.campbellcollaboration.org/resources/effect_size_input.php. Or Paul Ellis’s (2009) page: http://www.polyu.edu.hk/mm/effectsizefaqs/calculator/calculator.html. You can also use formulas in Microsoft Excel for inverting the t-distribution based on the p-values and the degrees of freedom: TINV(p;df). Remember from your statistics class that t = ES / SE.
If the workbooks of Meta-Essentials calculate effect sizes based on t-value or F-values (from ANOVAs), then the calculated effect size sometimes needs to be negative. Oftentimes, articles present only absolute values of t or F, and then the correct sign of the effect must be interpreted from information reported elsewhere in the article, such as in the discussion of the hypothesis. If the effect should be negative, a minus-sign can be added to the t or F-value inserted in the input tab of the workbooks.
If the sign of the effect size is wrong for other reasons, please check if you inserted the information in the correct order (what is group 1, what is group 2?) and check for typos.
Meta-analysts routinely correct for statistical artifacts as described in Hunter & Schmidt (2004), specifically when meta-analyzing correlation coefficients. Artifact corrections are proposed for reliability and imperfect construct validity, range restriction, and dichotomization of continuous variables. Researchers correct for these artifacts if they aim to provide a meta-analytical summary (i.e., combined effect size) of some relationship under ideal research circumstances. These corrections adjust the effect size upward (always) and this means the meta-analytical findings cannot easily be linked to any reported research finding.
The corrections can be applied before inserting the data (correlation coefficients) into workbook 5 of Meta-Essentials. This workbook does not provide automated calculation of adjusted effect sizes based on reliability estimates or other statistical artifacts. We emphasize that not everyone agrees such corrections are necessary (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p.109) and express our concern that only adjusting effect sizes upwards leads to unjustifiable large (and therefore likely statistically significant) combined effect sizes. Furthermore, insufficient data is often available from research reports to correct each effect size individually for their statistical artifacts, which is why many meta-analysts correct by using artifact distributions based on only the available data and assuming that this applies to data where it is unavailable as well.
Note that Hunter and Schmidt also propose different methods for meta-analyzing the effect sizes, but that Meta-Essentials follows the approach known as the Hedges-Olkin Meta-Analysis (HOMA).
Meta-analytical techniques
In Meta-Essentials the random effects model is used by default because the assumptions underlying the fixed effects model are very rarely met, especially in the social sciences. Furthermore, when a fixed effects model would make sense to use, i.e., when there is little variance in effect sizes, the random effects model automatically converges into a fixed effects model.
Confidence and prediction intervals
The Meta-Essentials software does not only generate a confidence interval for the combined effect size but additionally a ‘prediction interval’. Most other software for meta-analysis will not generate a prediction interval, although it is - in our view - the most essential outcome in a ‘random effects’ model, i.e. when it must be assumed that ‘true’ effect sizes vary. If a confidence level of 95% is chosen, the prediction interval gives the range in which, in 95% of the cases, the outcome of a future study will fall, assuming that the effect sizes are normally distributed (of both the included, and not (yet) included studies). This in contrast to the confidence interval, which “is often interpreted as indicating a range within which we can be 95% certain that the true effect lies. This statement is a loose interpretation, but is useful as a rough guide. The strictly-correct interpretation [… is that, i]f a study were repeated infinitely often, and on each occasion a 95% confidence interval calculated, then 95% of these intervals would contain the true effect.” (Schünemann, Oxman, Vist, Higgins, Deeks, Glasziou, & Guyatt, 2011, Section 12.4.1).
The confidence interval of the combined effect size is based on a t-distribution with k-1 degrees of freedom, k being the number of effect sizes. Therefore, if k decreases, the critical t-value to base the confidence interval increases. This reflects that a small number of studies does not give much certainty about the precision of the combined effect size. With more studies comes more confidence in the precision of the combined effect size.
At first, this may seem counterintuitive as you would expect that the confidence interval of the combined effect size would always be smaller and therefore give more precision regarding the combined effect size (particularly when individual effect sizes are very similar, i.e., homogeneous). However, assuming 95% confidence level, the 95% CI represents that range in which we can expect the combined effect to fall when the studies would be executed again. The imprecision includes, but is not limited to, sampling error and between-studies variance.
Microsoft Excel and calculations
The reason is that Excel recalculates every formula in the workbook each time a single new piece of data is inserted. To make your life easier, you can temporally set Excel to manually (and not automatically) calculate the workbooks, see the options in the ‘Formulas’ tab in the ribbon of Microsoft Excel. After you inserted all the data, you can either click ‘calculate now’ or set to automatically calculate.

Other topics
All the data inserted by default in the workbooks of Meta-Essentials is fictitious and were purposefully chosen to display some of the key features of the workbooks, such as the subgroup analysis. Thus, all the effect sizes, standard errors, subgroup variables, and moderator variables, were chosen by us. This data should be deleted before you start your own meta-analysis.
Textbooks on the entire process of a structured literature review:
- Cooper, H. (2009). Research synthesis and meta-analysis: A step-by-step approach (4th ed.). Los Angeles etc.: SAGE Publications.
- Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, London, New Delhi: SAGE Publications.
For more on the statistics behind meta-analysis:
- Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
- Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (Eds.). (2009). The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis (2nd Edition). New York: Russel Sage Foundation.
- Higgins, J. P. T., & Green, S. (Eds.). (n.d.). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (version 5.1.0). The Cochrane Collaboration.
For more information on the (mis)use of p-values, the usefulness of CIs, etc:
- Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the New Statistics. New York: Taylor & Francis Group.
- Cumming, G., & Finch, S. (2001). A Primer on the Understanding, Use, and Calculation of Confidence Intervals that are Based on Central and Noncentral Distributions. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61(4), 532–574.
- Cumming, G. (2013, September 22). Dance of the p-values. [video file] Retrieved from: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=5OL1RqHrZQ8
For more general information on effect sizes:
- Cumming, G. (n.d.) The New Statistics. [website] Retrieved from: http://www.thenewstatistics.com
- Ellis, P. (n.d.) Effect Size FAQs. [website] Retrieved from: https://effectsizefaq.com
Compared to version 1.0, version 1.1 only solved some bugs in the forest plot of the subgroup analysis (not all effect sizes were properly displayed).
Preferred citation
To cite the tools:
Suurmond, R, van Rhee, H, & Hak, T. (2017). Introduction, comparison, and validation of Meta-Essentials: a free and simple tool for meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods. 1-17. doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1260
To cite the user manual:
Van Rhee, H.J., Suurmond, R., & Hak, T. (2015). User manual for Meta-Essentials: Workbooks for meta-analyses (Version 1.0) Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Erasmus Research Institute of Management. Retrieved from www.erim.eur.nl/research-support/meta-essentials
To cite the text on interpreting results from meta-analysis:
Hak, T., Van Rhee, H. J., & Suurmond, R. (2016). How to interpret results of meta-analysis. (Version 1.0) Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Erasmus Rotterdam Institute of Management. Retrieved from: https://www.eur.nl/en/erim/meta-essentials
References
Aloë, A. M., & Becker, B. J. (2012). An Effect Size for Regression Predictors in Meta-Analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 37(2), 278–297. http://doi.org/10.3102/1076998610396901
Aloë, A. M. (2014). An Empirical Investigation of Partial Effect Sizes in Meta-Analysis of Correlational Data. The Journal of General Psychology, 141(1), 47–64. http://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2013.853021
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. doi.org/10.1002/9780470743386
Cheung, M. W.-L. (2015). Meta-Analysis: A Structural Equation Modeling Approach. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Cheung, M. W.-L., & Chan, W. (2005). Meta-analytic structural equation modeling: a two-stage approach. Psychological Methods. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/met/10/1/40/
Cheung, M. W.-L. (2015). metaSEM: an R package for meta-analysis using structural equation modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01521
Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the New Statistics. New York: Taylor & Francis Group.
Cumming, G., & Finch, S. (2001). A Primer on the Understanding, Use, and Calculation of Confidence Intervals that are Based on Central and Noncentral Distributions. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61(4), 532–574. http://doi.org/10.1177/0013164401614002
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: correcting error and bias in research findings (2nd, April). Thousand Oaks, London, New Delhi: SAGE Publications.
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, London, New Delhi: SAGE Publications.
Morris, S. B., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 105–125. doi.org/10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.105
Sánchez-Meca, J., & Marín-Martínez, F. (2008). Confidence intervals for the overall effect size in random-effects meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 13(1), 31–48. http://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.13.1.31
Schünemann, H., Oxman, A., et al. (2011). Interpreting results and drawing conclusions. In J. P. T. Higgins & S. G. Green (Eds.), Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. The Cochrane Collaboration. Retrieved from https://training.cochrane.org/handbook
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevB.91.121108